Database |
MEDLINE |
Publication Type |
Journal Article |
References |
Aldwin, C.M. (2007). Stress, Coping, and Development: an Integrative Perspective. N.Y. & London: Guilford Press. Aldwin, C.M., Sutton, K.J. & Lachman, M. (1996). ‘The development of coping resources in adulthood’. Journal of Personality, 64, 4, 837-71. Alexandrov, Yu. I. (2008). ‘How we fragment the world: the view from inside versus the view from outside’. Social Science Information. Special issue: Cognitive Technologies, 47, 3, 419-57. Alexandrov, Yu. I. (2015). ‘Cognition as systemogenesis’. In Anticipation: Learning from the Past: the Russian/Soviet Contributions to the Science of Anticipation, ed. M. Nadin (v. 25, pp. 193-220). Switzerland: Springer International Publishing. Alexandrov, Yu. I., Grinchenko, Yu. V., Shevchenko, D.G., Averkin, R.G., Matz, V.N., Laukka, S. & Korpusova, A.V. (2001). ‘A subset of cingulate cortical neurons is specifically activated during alcohol-acquisition behaviour’. Acta Physiologica Scandinavica, 171, 87-97. Alexandrov, Yu. I., Krylov, A.K. & Arutyunova, K.R. (2017). ‘Activity during learning and the nonlinear differentiation of experience’. Nonlinear Dynamics, Psychology, and Life Sciences, 21, 4, 391-405. Alexandrov, Yu. I., Sams, M., Lavikainen, J., Reinikainen, K. & Naatanen, R. (1998). ‘Differential effects of alcohol on the cortical processing of foreign and native language’. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 28, 1-10. Alexandrov, Yu. I. & Sams, M.E. (2005). ‘Emotion and consciousness: ends of a continuum’. Cognitive Brain Research, 25, 387-405. Alexandrov, Yu. I., Sozinov, A.A., Svarnik, O.E., Gorkin, A.G., Kuzina, E.A., Gavrilov, V.V. (2018). ‘Neuronal bases of systemic organization of behaviour’. Systems Neuroscience, Advances in Neurobiology, eds. A. Cheung-Hoi Yu, L. Li. 21, 1-33. Anokhin, P.K. (1974). A Biology and Neurophysiology of The Conditioned Reflex and Its Role in Adaptive Behaviour. Oxford: Pergamon Press. Anokhin, A.P., Birbaumer, N., Lutzenberger, W., Nikolaev, A. & Vogel, F. (1996). ‘Age increases brain complexity’. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 9, 63-68. Arutyunova, K.R., Alexandrov, Yu. I. (2016). ‘Acute alcohol administration decreases variation of moral judgments’. In Abstracts of the Seventh International Conference on Cognitive Science, pp. 25-26. Arutyunova, K.R., Alexandrov, Yu. I., Znakov, V.V. & Hauser, M.D. (2013). ‘Moral judgments in Russian culture: universality and cultural specificity’. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 13, 3-4, 255-85. Bakhchina, AV., Arutyunova, K.R., Sozinov, A.A., Demidovsky, A.V., Alexandrov Y.I. (2018). ‘Sample entropy of the heart rate reflects properties of the system organization of behaviour’. Entropy, 20, 6, 449. Barker, R.G., Dembo, T., Lewin, K. & Wright, M.E. (1947). ‘Experimental studies of frustration in young children’. In Readings in Social Psychology, eds. T.M. Newcomb & E.L. Hartley, (pp. 283-90). N.Y.: Holt. Barrett, L.F. (2006). ‘Are emotions natural kinds?’ Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 28-58. Barry, H. (1988). ‘Psychoanalytic theory of alcoholism’. In Theories on Alcoholism, eds. C.D. Chaudron & D.A. Wilkinson. (pp. 103-41). Toronto: Addiction Research Foundation. Barthol, R.P. & Ku, N.D. (1959). ‘Regression under stress to first learned behaviour’. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 59, 134-36. Bau, P.F.D., Moraes, R.S., Bau, C.H.D., Ferlin, E.L., Rosito, G.A. & Fuchs, F.D. (2011). ‘Acute ingestion of alcohol and cardiac autonomic modulation in healthy volunteers’. Alcohol, 45, 123-29. Bulava, A.I., Svarnik, O.E., Alexandrov, Yu. I. (2016). ‘Reconsolidation of previous memory: decreased cortical activity during acquisition of an active avoidance task as compared to an instrumental operant food-acquisition task’. Abstracts of 10th FENS Forum of Neuroscience, P044609. Carey, J.P. (2015). ‘Reinstatement of previously learned responses under conditions of extinction: a study of ‘regression’.’ Revista Mexicana de Análisis de la Conducta, 41, 14-18. Cohen, E.A. (1954). Human Behaviour in the Concentration Camps. London: Jonathan Cape. Conrod, P.J., Peterson, J.B., Pihl, R.O. (2001). ‘Reliability and validity of alcohol-induced heart rate increase as a measure of sensitivity to the stimulant properties of alcohol’. Psychopharmacology, 157, 20-30. Cushman, F., Young, L. & Hauser, M.D. (2006). ‘The role of conscious reasoning and intuition in moral judgments: testing three principles of harm’. Psychological Science, 17, 1082-89. De Kloet, E.R., Joëls, M. & Holsboer, F. (2005). ‘Stress and the brain: from adaptation to disease’. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 6, 463-75. Denton, K. & Krebs, D. (1990). ‘From the scene to the crime: the effect of alcohol and social context on moral judgment’. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 2, 242-48. Dominique, J.F., Roozendaal, B. & McGaugh, J.L. (1998). ‘Stress and glucocorticoids impair retrieval of long-term spatial memory’. Nature, 394, 787-90. Dudai, Y. (2012). ‘The restless engram: consolidations never end’. Annual Reviews in Neuroscience, 35, 227-47. Duke, А.А. & Begue, L. (2015). ‘The drunk utilitarian: blood alcohol concentration predicts utilitarian responses in moral dilemmas’. Cognition, 134, 121-27. Fraisse, P. & Piaget, J. (1968). Traité de Psychologie Expérimentale: Motivation, Emotion et Personnalité. Vol. 5. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France. Frankl, V. (1963). From Death Camp to Existentialism. N.Y.: Washington Square. Freud, S. (1905). Three Essays on the Theory of Sexuality. SE 7. Goldstein, K. (1939)/1995. The Organism: A Holistic Approach to Biology Derived from Pathological Data in Man. Salt Lake City: American Book Publishing. Hamilton, J.A. & Krechevsky, I. (1933). ‘Studies in the effect of shock upon behaviour plasticity in the rat’. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 16, 237-53. Heath, J. & Hardy-Valle’е, B. (2015). ‘Why do people behave immorally when drunk?’ Philosophical Explorations: An International Journal for the Philosophy of Mind and Action, 18, 3, 310-29. Heigl-Evers, A., Heigl, F., Ott, J. & Ruger, U. (1997). Lehrbuch der Psychotherapie. Gustav Fischer Verlag. Hidalgo, V., Pulopulos, M.M., Puig-Perez, S., Espin, L., Gomez-Amor, J., Salvador, A. (2015). Acute stress affects free recall and recognition of pictures differently depending on age and sex. Behavioural Brain Research, 292, 393-402. Isaychev, S.A., Chernorizov, A.M., Korolev, A.D., Isaychev, E.S., Dubynin, I.A. & Zakharov, I.A. (2012). ‘The psychophysiological diagnostics of the functional state of the athlete: preliminary data’. Psychology in Russia: State of the Art, 5, 1, 244-68. Jackson, J.H. (1958). Selected Writing of John Hughlings Jackson. N.Y.: Basic Books. James, W. (1890). The Principles of Psychology. London: MacMillan. Janet, P. (1928). ‘Fear of action as an essential element in the sentiment of melancholia’. In: Feelings and Emotions: The Wittenberg Symposium, ed. M.L. Reymert, (pp. 297-309). Worcester, MA: Clark University Press. Jung, C. (1953). Psychology and Alchemy. CW 13. Jung, C. (1961a). Critique of Psychoanalysis. N.Y.: Bollingen Foundation. Jung, C. (1961b). Freud and Psychoanalysis. Princeton: Princeton University Press. Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, Fast and Slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux. Kleemeier, R. W. (1942). Fixation and Regression in the Rat. Psychological Monographs, 54, 1-34. Kuhlmann, S., Piel, M., Wolf, O.T. (2005). Impaired memory retrieval after psychosocial stress in healthy young men. The Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 2977-2982. Laplanche, J. & Pontalis, J.B. (1984). Vocabulaire de la Psychanalyse. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France. Levi, P. (2007). I Sommersi e i Salvati. Turin: Einaudi. Lewin, K. (1937). ‘Psychoanalysis and topological psychology’. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 1, 202-12. Lewin, K. (1951). Field Theory in Social Science. New York: Harper. Lindquist, K.A., Siegel, E.H., Quigley, K.S., Barrett, L.F. (2013). ‘The hundred-year emotion war: are emotions natural kinds or psychological constructions? Comment on Lench, Flores and Bench (2011)’. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 255-63. Maier, S.U., Makwana, A.B. & Hare, T.A. (2015). ‘Acute stress impairs self-control in goal-directed choice by altering multiple functional connections within the brain’s decision circuits’. Neuron, 87, 3, 621-631. Margittai, Z., Strombach, T., van Wingerden, M., Joëls, M., Schwabe, L. & Kalenscher, T. (2015). ‘A friend in need: time-dependent effects of stress on social discounting in men’. Hormones and Behaviour, 73, 75-82. McEwen, B.S. (1998). ‘Stress, adaptation, and disease. Allostasis and allostatic load’. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 840, 33-44. Melillo, P., Bracale, M. & Pecchia, L. (2011). ‘Nonlinear heart rate variability features for real-life stress detection. Case study: students under stress due to university examination’. Biomedical Engineering Online, 10, 1, 96-109. Mowrer, O.H. (1940). ‘An experimental analogue of ‘regression’ with incidental observations on ‘reaction-formation’. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 35, 56-87. Paxton, J.M., Ungar, L. & Greene, J.D. (2012). ‘Reflection and reasoning in moral judgment’. Cognitive Science, 36, 1, 163-77. Peterson, J.B., Morey, J., Higgins, D.M. (2005). ‘You drink, I drink: alcohol consumption, social context and personality’. Individual Differences Research, 3, 23-35. Pincus, S.M. (1991). ‘Approximate entropy as a measure of system complexity’. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 88, 6, 2297-2301. Porter, E.H. & Biel, W. C. (1943). Alleged regressive behavior in a two-unit maze. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 35, 187-195. Rand, D. G., Greene, J. D. & Nowak, M. A. (2012). Spontaneous Giving and Calculated Greed. Nature, 489, 7416, 427-430. Ribot, T. (1901). Les Maladies de la Mémoire. Paris: Félix Alcan. Roozendaal, B., McEwen, B.S. & Chattarji, S. (2009). ‘Stress, memory and the amygdala’. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 423-33. Rycroft, C. (1995). A Critical Dictionary of Psychoanalysis. London: Penguin. Salvatore, S. (1972). The Influence of Sensory Pattern and Alcohol on Vehicular Velocity Sensing. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office. Salvatore, S. (1975). ‘Response speed as a function of sensory pattern and alcohol in a velocity judgment task’. Ergonomics, 18, 5, 491-502. Sanders, M.J. (1937). ‘An experimental demonstration of regression in the rat’. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 21, 493-510. Sartre, J.P. (2015). Sketch for a Theory of the Emotions. London: Routledge. Schacter, D.L. (1999). ‘The seven sins of memory. Insights from psychology and cognitive neuroscience’. American Psychologist, 54, 3, 182-203. Selye, H. (1936). ‘A syndrome produced by diverse nocuous agents’. Nature, 138, 3479, 32. Selye, H. (1973). ‘The evolution of the stress concept’. American Journal of Psychology, 61, 692-99. Shmalgauzen, I.I. (1949). Factors of Evolution: The Theory of Stabilizing Selection. Blakiston Co. Shvyrkov, V.B. (1986). ‘Behavioural specializations of neurons and system-selection hypothesis of learning’. In Human Memory and Cognitive Capabilities, eds. F. Klix & H. Hagendorf (pp. 599-611). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Sozinova, I.M., Sozinov, A.A., Seppo, J., Laukka, S.J., Alexandrov, Yu. I. (2017). ‘The prerequisites of prosocial behaviour in human ontogeny’. International Journal of Cognitive Research in Science, Engineering and Education, 5, 1, 57-63. Steckle, L.C. & O’Kelly, L.I. (1940). ‘The effect of electrical shock upon later learning and regression in the rat’. The Journal of Psychology, 9, 365-70. Steele, C.M., Critchlow, B. & Liu, T.J. (1985). ‘Alcohol and social behaviour II: the helpful drunkard’. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1, 35-46. Steele, C.M. & Josephs, R.A. (1990). ‘Alcohol myopia: its prized and dangerous effects’. American Psychologist, 45, 8, 921-33. Svarnik, O.E., Rusak, I.I., Bulava, A.I. & Alexandrov, Yu. I. (2016). ‘Acute alcohol administration before Task 2 learning diminishes Task 1 memory assemblies reactivation mapped by Fos expression. Program No. 825.02. 2016’. Neuroscience Meeting Planner. San Diego, CA: Society for Neuroscience, 2016. Online. Tononi, G. & Edelman, G.M. (1998). ‘Consciousness and complexity’. Science, 282, 1846-51. Valenza, G., Allegrini, P., Lanatà, A. & Scilingo, E.P. (2012). ‘Dominant Lyapunov exponent and approximate entropy in heart rate variability during emotional visual elicitation'. Frontiers in Neuroengineering, 5, 3, 1-7. Valsiner, J. (1992). ‘Making of the future: temporality and the constructive nature of human development’. In Time and Timing in Development, eds. G. Turkewitz & D. Devenney, (pp. 13-40). Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum. vonMonakow, C. (1925). The Emotions, Morality and the Brain. Washington D.C. & N.Y.: Nervous and Mental Disease Publishing Company. Wilkinson, M. (2010). Changing Minds in Therapy: Emotion, Attachment, Trauma and Neurobiology. N.Y. & London: W.W. Norton and Company. Wolf, O.T. (2009). ‘Stress and memory in humans: twelve years of progress?’ Brain Research, 1293, 142-54. Yu, R. (2016). ‘Stress potentiates decision biases: a stress induced deliberation-to-intuition (SIDI) model’. Neurobiology of Stress, 3, 83-95. Znamenskaya, I.I. (2018). ‘Attitudes toward out-group members: moral choice regression under acute stress’. In Abstracts of The Eighth International Conference on Cognitive Science, pp. 1271-72. |
Contributed Indexing |
Keywords: Alkohol; Entwicklung; Gen; Gewissensentscheidung; Hirn; Jungian psychology; Jungianische Psychologie; Krankheit; Neuron; Regression; Streß; System; alcohol; alcool; brain; cerebro; cerveau; cervello; choix moral; comportamento sociale; comportement social; conducta social; desarrollo; development; disease; développement; elección moral; enfermedad; esperienza individuale; estrés; experiencia individual; expérience individuelle; gene; gène; individual experience; maladie; malattia; moral choice; neuron; neurona; neurone; persönliche Erfahrung; psicologia junghiana; psicología Junguiana; psychologie jungienne; regresión; regression; regressione; régression; scelta morale; sistema; social behaviour; soziales Verhalten; stress; sviluppo; system; système; алкоголь; болезнь; ген; индивидуальный опыт; мозг; моральный выбор; нейрон; развитие; регрессия; система; социальное поведение; стресс; юнгианская психология; 荣格心理学, 退行, 酒精, 压力, 疾病, 社会行为, 个体经验, 发展, 系统, 神经元, 脑, 基因, 道德选择 Local Abstract: [Publisher, French] Le concept de régression est étudié en mettant l’accent sur les différences entre les positions de Freud et celles de Jung concernant sa portée. L’article discute les résultats des analyses expérimentales de dynamiques de l’expérience individuelle (de changements dans l’expression des gènes et de l’activité des réflexes neuronaux chez les animaux aux comportements pro-sociaux chez des humains en bonne santé et à des âges de vie divers, et chez des humains en situation de souffrance chronique) dans ces situations où se produit la régression: le stress, la maladie, l’apprentissage, les états hautement émotionnels et l’intoxication par l’alcool. Les mécanismes communs de régression dans toutes ces situations sont présentés. Les mécanismes de régression peuvent être décrits en tant que dé-différentiation réversible, ce qui est interprété comme un accroissement relatif de la représentation de systèmes peu-différenciés (plus vieux) dans l’expérience actualisée. Dans tous les cas de dé-différentiation mentionnés plus haut, la complexité de l’organisation systémique du comportement décroit de manière significative. [Publisher, German] Das Konzept der Regression wird unter besonderer Berücksichtigung der Unterschiede zwischen den Positionen von Freud und Jung auf seine Tragweite hin untersucht. Der Aufsatz diskutiert die Ergebnisse experimenteller Analysen der individuellen Erlebensdynamik (von Veränderungen der Genexpression und der impulsiven neuronalen Aktivität bei Tieren bis hin zu prosozialem Verhalten bei gesunden Menschen unterschiedlichen Alters und Menschen mit chronischen Schmerzen) in Situationen, in denen Regressionen auftreten: Streß, Krankheit, Lernen, sehr emotionale Zustände und Alkoholvergiftung. In all diesen Situationen werden gemeinsame Regressionsmechanismen angenommen. Diese Regressionsmechanismen können als reversible Entdifferenzierungen beschrieben werden, die als relativer Anstieg der Repräsentation niedrig differenzierter (älterer) Systeme in der aktualisierten Erfahrung verstanden wird. In allen oben genannten Fällen der Entdifferenzierung nimmt die Komplexität der systemischen Organisation des Verhaltens signifikant ab. [Publisher, Italian] Il concetto di regressione viene considerato con un’enfasi sulle differenze tra le posizioni di Freud e Jung riguardo al suo significato. L’articolo discute i risultati di analisi sperimentali delle dinamiche dell’esperienza individuale (dalle modificazioni nell’espressione genetica e dall’impulso dell'attività neuronale negli animali fino al comportamento prosociale in esseri umani sani in età differenti, e in esseri umani con dolore cronico) in quelle situazioni in cui si verifica la regressione: stress, malattia, apprendimento, stati altamente emotivi e intossicazione alcolica. Vengono proposti meccanismi comuni di regressione in tutte queste situazioni. I meccanismi di regressione possono essere descritti come dedifferenziazione reversibile, che è intesa come un aumento relativo della rappresentazione di sistemi a bassa differenziazione (più vecchi) nell’esperienza attualizzata. In tutti i casi di dedifferenziazione sopra menzionati, la complessità dell’organizzazione sistemica del comportamento diminuisce significativamente. [Publisher, Russian] Понятие регрессии рассматривается в свете различий взглядов Фрейда и Юнга на значение регрессии. В статье обсуждаются результаты экспериментального анализа динамики индивидуального опыта (начиная от изменений генной экспрессии и спайковой активности нейронов у животных до просоциального поведения у здоровых людей в разные возрастные периоды и людей, испытывающих хроническую боль) в ситуациях, в которых происходит регрессия: стресс, болезнь, обучение, состояния сильного эмоционального возбуждения, алкогольная интоксикация. Обосновывается представление об общности механизмов регрессии во всех этих ситуациях. Механизмы регрессии могут быть описаны как обратимая дедифференциация, под которой понимается относительное увеличение представленности низко-дифференцированных (старых) систем в актуализированном опыте. Во всех случаях упомянутой выше дедифференциации, сложность системной организации поведения существенно снижается. [Publisher, Spanish; Castilian] Se considera el concepto de regresión, con énfasis en las diferencias entre Freud y Jung en lo que concierne a su significado. El trabajo desarrolla los resultados de análisis experimentales sobre dinámicas experienciales individuales (desde cambios en la expresión genética y actividad neuronal en animales a conductas prosociales en humanos saludables en diferentes edades, y humanos en dolor crónico) en aquellas situaciones donde la regresión ocurre: estrés, enfermedad, aprendizaje, estados altamente emocionales e intoxicación alcohólica. Se proponen mecanismos comunes de regresión en todas estas situaciones. Los mecanismos de regresión pueden describirse como de-diferenciación reversible, la cual es comprendida como un incremento relativo de la representación de sistemas de baja-diferenciación (antiguos) en la experiencia actualizada. En todos los casos de de-diferenciación mencionados anteriormente, la complejidad de la organización sistemática de conducta decrece significativamente. [Publisher, Chinese] 退行I:退行的实证取向 退行的概念被认为凸显了弗洛伊德和荣格关于退行的重要性的不同理解。文章讨论了关于个体处于退行发生的情境时, 其经验动力的实证分析的结果 (这些分析涉及动物基因表达的改变和神经元脉冲活性, 以及不同年龄健康人类亲社会的行为, 和慢性疼痛的病人), 这些导致退行的情境包括:压力, 疾病, 学习, 高情绪化状态, 酒精中毒。这些情境下常见的退行机制都在文中提及。退行的机制可以被描述为可逆的去分化, 这可以被理解为在实现经验中, 低分化的 (老一些的)系统表现出的相对增长。在所有以上提及的去分化案例中, 系统化组织的行为的复杂性显著降低了。. |
DOI |
10.1111/1468-5922.12580 |
PMID |
32170745 |
|